Ознакомительная версия.
Billari F. C., Prskawetz A., Fent T., Aparicio Diaz B. (2007). The «Wedding-Ring»: An agent-based marriage model based on social interaction. Demographic Research 17, 59–82.
Bingham C. B., and Eisenhardt K. M. (2011). Rational heuristics: The «simple rules» that strategists learn from process experience. Strategic Management Journal 32, 1437–1464.
Bomlitz L. J., Brezis M. (2008). Misrepresentation of health risks by mass media. Journal of Public Health 30, 202–204.
Bond M. (2009). Risk school. Nature 461, 1189–1192.
Bower B. (2011, June 4). Simple heresy. Science News 179, 26–29.
Bramwell R., West H., Salmon P. (2006). Health professionals’ and service users’ interpretation of screening test results: Experimental study. British Medical Journal 333, 284–286.
Brandt A. M. (2007). The cigarette century. New York: Basic Books.
Brenner D. J. (2010). Slowing the increase in the population dose resulting from CT scans. Radiation Research 174, 809–815.
Brenner D. J., Elliston C. D. (2004). Estimated radiation risk potentially associated with full-body CT screening. Radiology 232, 735–738.
Brenner D. J., Hall E. J. (2007). Computed tomography – An increasing source of radiation exposure. New England Journal of Medicine 357, 2277–2284.
Chen X.-Y. (2007). Defensive medicine or economically motivated corruption? A Confucian reflection on physician care in China today. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 32, 635–648.
Chou R. et al. (2005). Screening for HIV: A review of the evidence for the U. S. Preventive Services Task Force. Annals of Internal Medicine 143, 55–73.
Cohen D., and Carter P. (2010). WHO and the pandemic flu «conspiracies». British Medical Journal 340, 1274–1279.
Cohen I. B. (1998). Howard Aiken on the number of computers needed for the nation. IEEE Annals of the History of Computing 20, 27–32.
Covey J. (2007). A meta-analysis of the effects of presenting treatment benefits in different formats. Medical Decision Making 27, 638–654.
Cosmides L., and Tooby J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition 58, 1–73.
Czerlinski J., Gigerenzer G., Goldstein D. G. (1999). How good are simple heuristics? In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, the ABC Research Group, Simple heuristics that make us smart (p. 97–118). New York: Oxford University Press.
Daston L. J. (1988). Classical probability in the Enlightenment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Doll R., Peto R., Boreham J., Sutherland I. (2004). Mortality in relation to smoking: 50 years’ observations on male British doctors. British Medical Journal 328, 1519.
Delongchamps N. B., Sing A., Haas G. P. (2006). The role of prevalence in the diagnosis of prostate cancer. Cancer Control 13, 158–168.
DeMiguel V., Garlappi L., Uppal R. (2009). Optimal versus naive diversification: How inefficient is the 1/ N portfolio strategy? Review of Financial Studies 22, 1915–1953.
Dewey J. (1929). The quest for certainty. New York: Minton, Balch and Co.
Dijksterhuis A., van Knippenberg A., Kruglanski A. W., Schaper C. (1996). Motivated social cognition: Need for closure effects on memory and judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 32, 254–270.
DiPrete T. A. (2007). Is this a great country? Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 25, 89–95.
Djulbegovic M., Beyth R. J., Neuberger M. M., Stoffs T. L., Vieweg J., Djulbegovic B., Dahm P. (2010). Screening for prostate cancer: Systematic review and meta analysis of randomized controlled trials. British Medical Journal 341, c4543.
Doll R., Peto R., Boreham J., Sutherland I. (2004). Mortality in relation to smoking: 50 years’ observations on male British doctors. British Medical Journal 328, 1519.
Domenighetti G., Casabianca A., Gutzwiller F., and Martinoli S. (1993). Revisiting the most informed consumer of surgical services: The physician-patient. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care 9, 505–513.
Donner-Banzhoff N., Bastian H., Coulter A., Elwyn G., Jonitz G., Klemperer D., Ludwig W. D. (2011). How can better evidence be delivered? In G. Gigerenzer, J. A. Muir Gray (Eds.), Better doctors, better patients, better decisions (p. 215–232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Doshi P. (2009). Neuraminidase inhibitors: The story behind the Cochrane review. British Medical Journal 330, 1348–1351.
Egidi M., and Marengo L. (2004). Near-decomposability, organization, and evolution: Some notes on Herbert Simon’s contribution. In M. Augier and J. J. March (Eds.), Models of a man: Essays in memory of Herbert A. Simon (p. 335–350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ehrenreich B. (2010). Bright-sided: How positive thinking is undermining America. New York: Picador.
Einstein A. (undated). Letter to Max Wertheimer. Albert Einstein Archive, Princeton University Library.
Emanuel E. J. (2008). Healthcare, guaranteed: A simple, secure solution for America. New York: Public Affairs.
Enserink M. (2010, December 24). What’s next for disease eradication? Science 330, 1736–1739. doi: 10.1126/ science. 330.6012.1736.
Finkel E. J., Eastwick P. W., Karney B. R., Reis H. T., Sprecher S. (2012). Online dating: A critical analysis from the perspective of psychological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 13, 3–66.
Firman D., Quirk K. (2009). Brace for impact. Deerfield Beach, FL: Health Communications, Inc.
Flegal K. M., Graubard B. I., Williamson D. F., Gail M. H. (2005). Excess deaths associated with underweight, overweight, and obesity. Journal of the American Medical Association 293, 861–1867.
Folkman J., Kalluri R. (2004). Cancer without disease. Nature 427, 787. doi: 10. 1038 /427787a
Franklin B. (1745). Letter to a friend (Philadelphia, June 25, 1745). Retrieved from www.bibliomania.com/2/9/77/124/21473/1/frameset.html.
Franklin B. (1779). Letter to Jonathan Williams (Passy, April 8, 1779). In A. H. Smyth (Ed.), The writings of Benjamin Franklin (Vol. VII. p. 281–282). New York: Macmillan.
Friedman D. (2004). Monty Hall’s three doors: Construction and deconstruction of a choice anomaly. American Economic Review 88, 933–946.
Friedman D., Nakhoda A. (2008, August). Monty Hall Problem. Learning and Experimental Economics Projects of Santa Cruz (LEEPS), University of Santa Cruz. Retrieved from leeps.ucsc.edu/misc/page/monty-hall-puzzle/.
Furedi A. (1999). The public health implications of the 1995 «pill scare». Human Reproduction Update 5, 621–626. doi:10.1093/humupd/5.6. 621.
Gaissmaier W. Gigerenzer G. (2012). 9/ 11, act II: A fine-grained analysis of regional variations in traffic fatalities in the aftermath of the terrorist attacks. Psychological Science 23, 1449–1454.
Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R. (2012). The risks we dread: A social circle account. PLoS ONE, 7, e32837.
Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R., Gigerenzer G. (2009). Using icon arrays to communicate medical risks to low-numeracy people. Health Psychology 28,
210–216.
Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R., Gigerenzer G. (2014). Maximizing and satisficing: Personality trait or adaptive strategies? Manuscript, Max Planck Institute for Human Development: Berlin.
Garcia-Retamero R., Galesic M. (2012). Doc, what would you do if you were me? On self-other discrepancies in medical decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 18, 38–51.
Gaskell G., Allansdottir A., Allum N., Corchero C., Fischler C., Hampel, J., et al. (2006). Europeans and biotechnology in 2005: Pattern and trends. Eurobarometer 64.3.
Gawande A. (2009). The checklist manifesto. New York: Metropolitan Books.
Ghosh A. K., Ghosh K. (2005). Translating evidence-based information into effective risk communication: Current challenges and opportunities. Journal of Laboratory and Clinical Medicine 145, 171–180. doi:10.1016/ j.lab.2005.02.006.
Gibson R., Singh J. P. (2010). The treatment trap. Chicago, IL: Dee.
Gigerenzer G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer G. (2002). Calculated risks: How to know when numbers deceive you. New York: Simon and Schuster (UK version: Reckoning with risk: Learning to live with uncertainty. London: Penguin).
Gigerenzer G. (2004). Dread risk, September 11, and fatal traffic accidents. Psychological Science 15, 286–287 doi:10.1111/j.0956–7976.200 4.00668.x.
Gigerenzer G. (2006). Out of the frying pan into the fire: Behavioral reactions to terrorist attacks. Risk Analysis 26, 347–351. doi:10.1111/ j.1539–6924.2006.00753.x.
Gigerenzer G. (2007). Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious. New York: Viking (UK version: London: Allen Lane/Penguin).
Gigerenzer G. (2008). Rationality for mortals: How people cope with uncertainty. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer G. (2011). What are natural frequencies? Doctors need to find better ways to communicate risk to patients. British Medical Journal 343: d6386.
Gigerenzer G., Brighton H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science 1, 107–143.
Gigerenzer G., Fiedler K., Olsson H. (2012). Rethinking cognitive biases as environmental consequences. In P. M. Todd, G. Gigerenzer, the ABC Research Group, Ecological rationality: Intelligence in the world (p. 80–110). New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer G., Gaissmaier W., Kurz-Milcke E., Schwartz L. M., Woloshin S. (2007). Helping doctors and patients to make sense of health statistics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 8, 53–96.
Gigerenzer G., Galesic M. (2012). Why do single event probabilities confuse patients? British Medical Journal 344, e245. doi:0.1136/ bmj.e245.
Gigerenzer G., Galesic M., Garcia-Retamero R. (2013). Stereotypes about men’s and women’s intuitions: A study of two nations. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology. doi:10.1177/0022022113487074.
Gigerenzer G., Goldstein D. G. (2011). The recognition heuristic: A decade of research. Judgment and Decision Making 6, 100–121.
Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R., van den Broek E., Fasolo B., Katsikopoulos K. V. (2005). «A 30 % chance of rain tomorrow»: How does the public understand probabilistic weather forecasts? Risk Analysis 25, 623–629.
Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R., and Pachur T. (Eds.) (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer G., Mata, J., Frank R. (2009). Public knowledge of benefits of breast and prostate cancer screening in Europe. Journal of the National Cancer Institute 101(17), 1216–1220. doi:10.1093/jnci/djp237.
Gigerenzer G., Muir Gray J. A. (Eds.) (2011). Better doctors, better patients, better decisions: Envisioning health care 2020. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gigerenzer G., Selten R. (2001). Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gigerenzer G., Swijtink Z., Porter T., Daston L., Beatty J., Krüger L. (1989). The empire of chance: How probability changed science and everyday life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gigerenzer G., Todd P. M., the ABC Research Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gigerenzer G., Wegwarth O., Feufel M. (2010). Misleading communication of risk: Editors should enforce transparent reporting in abstracts. British Medical Journal 341, 791–792. doi:10.1136/ bmj.c4830.
Goldstein D. G., Gigerenzer G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review 109, 75–90. doi:10.1037/ 0033–295X.109.1.75.
Goldstein D. G., and Taleb N. N. (2007). We don’t quite know what we are talking about when we talk about volatility. Journal of Portfolio Management 33, 84–86.
Good Stewardship Working Groups (2011). The «top 5» lists in primary care. Archives of Internal Medicine 171, 385–390.
Gøtzsche P. C., Nielsen M. (2011). Screening for breast cancer with mammography. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 1, Article CD001877. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD001877. pub4.
Gould S. J. (1992). Bully for brontosaurus: Further reflections in natural history. New York: Penguin Books.
Graham I. D., Stiell I. G., Laupacis A., et al. (2001). Awareness and use of the Ottawa ankle and knee rules in 5 countries: Can publication alone be enough to change practice? Annals of Emergency Medicine 37, 259–266.
Gurian E. A., Kinnamon D. D., Henry J. J., Waisbren S. E. (2006). Expanded new-born screening for biomedical disorders: The effect of a false-positive result. Pediatrics 117, 1915–1921.
Haag L., Stern E. (2003). In search of the benefits of learning Latin. Journal of Educational Psychology 95, 174–178.
Hair M., Renaud K. V., Ramsay J. (2007). The influence of self-esteem and locus of control on perceived email-related stress. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 2791–2803.
Haldane A. G. (2012). The dog and the Frisbee. Speech, Jackson Hole, August 31, 2012. www.bankofengland.co.uk/publications/Pages/speeches/2012/596.aspx.
Hale W. R. (1996). Can the Ottawa ankle decision rules be applied in the United States? Western Journal of Medicine 164, 363.
Harris R., and Lohr K. N. (2002). Screening for prostate cancer: An update of the evidence for the U. S. Preventive Services Task Force. Annals of Internal Medicine 137, 917–929.
Ознакомительная версия.